Journal Home > Volume 8 , Issue 1
Background

Birds produce alarm calls to convey information about threats. Some Passerine alarm calls consist of several note strings, but few studies have examined their function. Previous studies have shown that Japanese Tits (Parus minor) can alter the calling rate and number and combination of notes in response to predators. We previously found the combinations of note types in Japanese Tit alarm calls to be significantly different in response to the Sparrowhawk (Accipiter nisus) and Common Cuckoo (Cuculus canorus).

Methods

Through playback experiments, we tested whether the note strings in Japanese Tit alarm calls to the Common Cuckoo have different functions in conveying information. The note strings of selected alarm calls were divided into the categories of C and D, and different calls were then constructed separately based on the two note string categories. Original alarm calls (C-D), C calls and D calls were played back to male Japanese Tits during the incubation period.

Results

Male Japanese Tits had a significantly stronger response to C calls than to C-D calls, and they showed a significantly stronger response to both C and C-D calls than to D calls, suggesting that Japanese Tits discriminated between the C and D calls.

Conclusions

Our study demonstrated that the C- and D-category note strings of Japanese Tit alarm calls to the Common Cuckoo have different functions, which supports the previous finding that different note strings in an alarm call can provide different information to receivers. However, the exact meanings of these note strings are not yet known, and further investigation is therefore required.


menu
Abstract
Full text
Outline
About this article

Function of note strings in Japanese Tit alarm calls to the Common Cuckoo: a playback experiment

Show Author's information Jiangping Yu1Weiwei Lv2Hongwei Xu1Nehafta Bibi1Yangyang Yu1Yunlei Jiang1,3Wei Liang4Haitao Wang1,2( )
Jilin Provincial Engineering Laboratory of Avian Ecology and Conservation Genetics, School of Life Sciences, Northeast Normal University, Changchun 130024, China
Jilin Key Laboratory of Animal Resource Conservation and Utilization, Northeast Normal University, Changchun 130024, China
Animal Science and Technology College, Jilin Agricultural University, Xincheng Street 2888, Changchun 130118, China
Ministry of Education Key Laboratory for Ecology of Tropical Islands, College of Life Sciences, Hainan Normal University, Haikou 571158, China

Abstract

Background

Birds produce alarm calls to convey information about threats. Some Passerine alarm calls consist of several note strings, but few studies have examined their function. Previous studies have shown that Japanese Tits (Parus minor) can alter the calling rate and number and combination of notes in response to predators. We previously found the combinations of note types in Japanese Tit alarm calls to be significantly different in response to the Sparrowhawk (Accipiter nisus) and Common Cuckoo (Cuculus canorus).

Methods

Through playback experiments, we tested whether the note strings in Japanese Tit alarm calls to the Common Cuckoo have different functions in conveying information. The note strings of selected alarm calls were divided into the categories of C and D, and different calls were then constructed separately based on the two note string categories. Original alarm calls (C-D), C calls and D calls were played back to male Japanese Tits during the incubation period.

Results

Male Japanese Tits had a significantly stronger response to C calls than to C-D calls, and they showed a significantly stronger response to both C and C-D calls than to D calls, suggesting that Japanese Tits discriminated between the C and D calls.

Conclusions

Our study demonstrated that the C- and D-category note strings of Japanese Tit alarm calls to the Common Cuckoo have different functions, which supports the previous finding that different note strings in an alarm call can provide different information to receivers. However, the exact meanings of these note strings are not yet known, and further investigation is therefore required.

Keywords: Brood parasitism, Playback, Alarm call, Male Japanese Tit, Note strings, Function

References(44)

Bates D. lme4: Mixed-effects modelling with R. 2010. http://lme4.r-forge.r-project.org/book/.

Canestrari D, Marcos JM, Baglione V. Cooperative breeding in carrion crows reduces the rate of brood parasitism by great spotted cuckoos. Anim Behav. 2009;77:1337-44.

Caro T. Antipredator defences in birds and mammals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2005.
Davies NB. Cuckoos, cowbirds and other cheats. London: T and A D Poyser; 2000.

Davies NB. Cuckoo adaptations: trickery and tuning. J Zool. 2011;284:1-14.

Davies NB, Welbergen JA. Cuckoo-hawk mimicry? An experimental test. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2008;275:1817-22.

Freeberg TM. Complexity in the chick-a-dee call of Carolina chickadees (Poecile Carolinensis): associations of context and signaler behavior to call structure. Auk. 2008;125:896-907.

Gill SA, Bierema AMK. On the meaning of alarm calls: a review of functional reference in avian alarm calling. Ethology. 2013;119:449-61.

Gill SA, Sealy SG. Functional reference in an alarm signal given during nest defence: seet calls of yellow warblers denote brood-parasitic brown-headed cowbirds. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2004;56:71-80.

Griesser M. Referential calls signal predator behavior in a group-living bird species. Curr Biol. 2008;18:69-73.

Grim T, Rutila J, Cassey P, Hauber ME. The cost of virulence: an experimental study of egg eviction by brood parasitic chicks. Behav Ecol. 2009;20:1138-46.

Grim T, Samaš P, Procházka P, Rutila J. Are tits really unsuitable hosts for the common cuckoo? Ornis Fennica. 2014;91:166-77.

Hetrick SA, Sieving KE. Antipredator calls of tufted titmice and interspecific transfer of encoded threat information. Behav Ecol. 2011;23:83-92.

Hurd CR. Interspecific attraction to the mobbing calls of black capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1996;38:287-92.

Langham GM, Contreras TA, Sieving KE. Why pishing works: Titmouse (Paridae) scolds elicit a generalized response in bird communities. Ecoscience. 2006;13:485-96.

Langmore NE, Kilner RM, Butchart SHM, Maurer G, Davies NB, Cockburn A, Macgregor NA, Peters A, Magrath MJL, Dowling DK. The evolution of egg rejection by cuckoo hosts in Australia and Europe. Behav Ecol. 2005;16:686-92.

Leavesley AJ, Magrath RD. Communicating about danger: urgency alarm calling in a bird. Anim Behav. 2005;70:365-73.

Liang W, Møller AP. Hawk mimicry in cuckoos and anti-parasitic aggressive behavior of barn swallows in Denmark and China. J Avian Biol. 2015;46:216-23.

Liang W, Møller AP, Stokke BG, Yang C, Kovařík P, Wang H, Yao C-T, Ding P, Lu X, Moksnes A, Røskaft E, Grim T. Geographic variation in egg ejection rate by great tits across 2 continents. Behav Ecol. 2016;27:1405-12.

Lima SL. Predators and the breeding bird: behavioral and reproductive flexibility under the risk of predation. Biol Rev. 2009;84:485-513.

Matysioková B, Remeš V. Incubation feeding and nest attentiveness in a socially monogamous songbird: role of feather colouration, territory quality and ambient environment. Ethology. 2010;116:596-607.

Moksnes A, Røskaft E, Braa AT, Korsnes L, Lampe HM, Pedersen HC. Behavioural responses of potential hosts towards artificial cuckoo eggs and dummies. Behaviour. 1991;116:64-89.

Møller AP, Stokke BG, Samia D. Hawk models, hawk mimics and anti-predator behavior of prey. Behav Ecol. 2015;26:1039-44.

Pinheiro JC, Bates D. Mixed effects models in S and S-Plus. New York: Springer; 2000.https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-0318-1
DOI

Rice WR. Analyzing tables of statistical tests. Evolution. 1989;43:223-5.

Rothstein SI, Robinson SK. Parasitic birds and their hosts: studies in coevolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1998.

Soard CM, Ritchison G. "Chick-a-dee" calls of Carolina chickadees convey information about degree of threat posed by avian predators. Anim Behav. 2009;78:1447-53.

Suzuki TN. Referential mobbing calls elicit different predator-searching behaviours in Japanese great tits. Anim Behav. 2012;84:53-7.

Suzuki TN. Communication about predator type by a bird using discrete, graded and combinatorial variation in alarm calls. Anim Behav. 2014;87:59-65.

Suzuki TN. Assessment of predation risk through referential communication in incubating birds. Sci Rep. 2015;5:10239.

Suzuki TN, Ueda K. Mobbing calls of Japanese tits signal predator type: field observations of natural predator encounters. Wilson J Ornithol. 2013;125:412-5.

Suzuki TN, Wheatcroft D, Griesser M. Experimental evidence for compositional syntax in bird calls. Nat Commun. 2016;7:10986.

Templeton CN, Greene E. Nuthatches eavesdrop on variations in heterospecific chickadee mobbing alarm calls. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104:5479-82.

Templeton CN, Greene E, Davis K. Allometry of alarm calls: black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science. 2005;308:1934-8.

van Balen JH, Booy CJH, Van Franeker JA, Osieck ER. Studies on hole-nesting birds in natural nest sites. 1. Availability and occupation of natural nest sites. Ardea. 1982;70:1-24.

van Duyse E, Pinxten R, Eens M. Effects of testosterone on song, aggression, and nestling feeding behavior in male great tits, Parus major. Horm Behav. 2002;41:178-86.

Welbergen JA, Davies NB. Reed warblers discriminate cuckoos from sparrowhawks with graded alarm signals that attract males and neighbours. Anim Behav. 2008;76:811-22.

Wheatcroft D, Price TD. Learning and signal copying facilitate communication among bird species. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2013;280:20123070.

Wheatcroft D, Price TD. Rates of signal evolution are associated with the nature of interspecific communication. Behav Ecol. 2015;26:83-90.

Wilson DR, Mennill DJ. Duty cycle, not signal structure, explains conspecific and heterospecific responses to the calls of black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus). Behav Ecol. 2011;22:784-90.

Yang C, Liang W, Antonov A, Cai Y, Stokke BG, Fossøy F, Moksnes A, Røskaft E. Diversity of parasitic cuckoos and their hosts in China. Chin Birds. 2012;3:9-32.

You Y, Feng J, Wang H, Wang J, Dong C, Su X, Sun H, Gao W. Variation in egg size and nestling growth rate in relation to clutch size and laying sequence in great tits Parus major. Prog Nat Sci. 2009;19:427-33.

Yu J, Wang L, Xing X, Yang C, Ma J, Møller AP, Wang H, Liang W. Barn swallows (Hirundo rustica) differentiate between common cuckoo and sparrowhawk in China: alarm calls convey information on threat. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2016;70:171-8.

Yu J, Xing X, Jiang Y, Liang W, Wang H, Møller AP. Alarm call-based discrimination between common cuckoo and Eurasian sparrowhawk in a Chinese population of great tits. Ethology. 2017;123:542-50.

Publication history
Copyright
Acknowledgements
Rights and permissions

Publication history

Received: 19 December 2016
Accepted: 16 August 2017
Published: 30 August 2017
Issue date: January 2017

Copyright

© The Author(s) 2017.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Qianxi Fan, Dianzheng Zhao, Zhenni Liu, Siyu Zhang, Songhao Li, Yifu Sun for their assistance in fieldwork. We also thank the Zuojia Nature Reserve for the support and permission to carry out this study. This work is supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31272331 and 31470458 to HW, 31472013 and 31772453 to WL), the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (2412016KJ043) and the Open Project Program of Jilin Provincial Key Laboratory of Animal Resource Conservation and Utilization (130028685).

Rights and permissions

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Return